تعداد نشریات | 25 |
تعداد شمارهها | 932 |
تعداد مقالات | 7,652 |
تعداد مشاهده مقاله | 12,494,898 |
تعداد دریافت فایل اصل مقاله | 8,886,369 |
تاثیر سلسلهمراتب ردهشناختی شخص و شمار در تحول تاریخی ضمیرهای انعکاسی فارسی نو | ||
زبان پژوهی | ||
مقاله 4، دوره 13، شماره 40، مهر 1400، صفحه 89-114 اصل مقاله (339.85 K) | ||
نوع مقاله: مقاله پژوهشی | ||
شناسه دیجیتال (DOI): 10.22051/jlr.2020.30695.1851 | ||
نویسندگان | ||
زهرا اعتباری1؛ علی علیزاده* 2؛ مهرداد نغزگوی کهن3 | ||
1دانشجوی دکتری زبانشناسی همگانی، گروه زبانشناسی، دانشگاه فردوسی مشهد | ||
2دکترای تخصصی زبانشناسی، دانشیار گروه زبانشناسی، هیأت علمی دانشگاه فردوسی مشهد | ||
3دکترای تخصصی زبانشناسی، دانشیار گروه زبانشناسی، هیأت علمی دانشگاه بوعلی سینا همدان | ||
چکیده | ||
این مقاله، به بررسی تحول تاریخی ضمایر انعکاسیِ مبتنی بر «خود»، در زبان فارسیِ نو میپردازد. همراهی ضمایر شخصی پیبستی با «خود» در همۀ قرنهای این دوره مشاهده نشدهاست. بر این مبنا، پژوهش حاضر بر آن است که از منظر ردهشناختی و با بهرهگیری از سلسلهمرتبههای جهانی نمایهسازی شخص و شمار، به بررسی ترتیب ظهور و چگونگی تحول این ضمایر بپردازد. به این منظور، پیکرهای منتخب از سه متن روایی از هر یک از یازده قرن این دوره تهیه شد و جملههای دربردارندة ضمایر انعکاسی شخصی استخراج گردید. فراوانی رخدادِ شخصها و شمارهای مختلف این ضمایر و همچنین فراوانی نقشهای گوناگون آنها در هر قرن در جداولی نمایش داده شدهاست. بر مبنای پیکرۀ مورد بررسی، نخستین نشانۀ همراهی ضمیر شخصی با «خود» در قرن هشتم هجری اتفاق افتادهاست. همچنین بررسی پیکره نشان میدهد که همراهی ضمایر شخصی پیبستی با «خود» چه از منظر ترتیب ظهور و چه فراوانی رخداد دارای اولویت سومشخص بر اولشخص و اول بر دومشخص است و در نتیجه جانداری نقشی در این تحول ندارد. همچنین در مشخصۀ شمار اولویت با شمار مفرد است. افزون بر این، نخستین همراهی ضمایر پیبستی با «خود» در نقش ملکی و پس از آن در سایر تأکیدیها و سپس انعکاسی بودهاست. | ||
کلیدواژهها | ||
ردهشناسی تاریخی؛ ضمایر انعکاسی؛ تاکیدی؛ واژهبست ضمیری؛ فارسی نو | ||
عنوان مقاله [English] | ||
Effects of person and number hierarchies on development of personal reflexive pronouns in New Persian | ||
نویسندگان [English] | ||
Zahra Etebari1؛ Ali Alizadeh2؛ Mehrdad Naghzgouy- Kohan3 | ||
1Department of Linguistics- Ali Shariati Faculty of Literature and Humanities- Feredowsi Unversity of Mashhad- Mashhad- Iran | ||
2Associate professor- Department of Linguistics- Ali Shariati Faculty of Literature and Humanities- Ferdowsi University of Mashhad- Mashhad- Iran | ||
3PhD in Linguistics, Associate Professor, Department of Linguistics, Faculty of Bu Ali Sina University, Hamadan | ||
چکیده [English] | ||
INTRODUCTION This article studies the diachronic development of reflexive pronouns derived from xod in New Persian, i.e. 10th to 20th centuries. Not in all centuries of New Persian one can find the indexation of xod by pronominal clitics and it is mostly believed that the indexation is a characteristic of colloquial Persian. The current study, employing a typological perspective, tries to investigate the sequence of the emergence as well as the development process of the reflexive pronouns with different persons and numbers in New Persian. We assume that the process follows the universal referential hierarchies. In Persian, similar to for example English, Turkish and Finnish, one and a same set of reflexive pronouns show both reflexive and emphatic functions. The reflexive pronoun xod in New Persian is derived from xwad in Middle Persian with adverbial function (Mackenzie, 2011), and xwad is a development from the prefix uva with possessive meaning in Old Persian (Kent, 1950). New Persian uses xod as both emphatic and reflexive pronouns. Moyne (1971) considers possessive function of xod as emphatic. However, Davari (2016) points out that the use of xod as a possessor is to denote reflexive function and based on this concludes that the reflexive uses of xod in Persian precede the emphatic uses. Regarding person and number indexations of different grammatical categories, different hierarchies are introduced (Greenberg, 1966; Comrie, 1989, 1999; Woolford, 1999; Corbett, 2000; Croft, 2003). In some hierarchies (according to certain languages), animacy plays a bigger role, dominating first and second persons over third person. However, for some languages, animacy is indifferent and they follow the hierarchy with the domination of third person. Furthermore, specifically for the presence of reflexive markers with different persons among languages, Comrie (1989; 1999) introduces the hierarchy that dominates third person over first and second persons, and Faltz (1985) believes that second person also dominates first person in reflexive pronouns. This study aims to determine what referential hierarchies play role in the emergence and development of the reflexive pronoun xod into personal reflexive pronouns xodam, xodat, xodaš, xodemān, xodetān, and xodešān. It also takes a brief look at the influence of different functions of xod in the process. MATERIALS AND METHODS Since the study is diachronic, the given data at first place are of written type and only the prose of this period is investigated. Among various available texts, three manuscripts are selected per century, in total 33 texts. The criteria of the selections are the simplicity of the language of the texts, the narrativity of the texts, and the certainty on the dates they were written. From each text, 15000 words are compiled which resulted in a half a million word corpus[1]. The corpus includes 192 constructions where the reflexive pronoun xod is used with one of the pronominal clitics. The current article takes a diachronic typological approach and regarding the frequencies of the personal reflexive pronouns in different centuries of New Persian, investigates the influence of different referential hierarchies (Greenberg, 1966; Faltz, 1985; Comrie, 1989a, 1989b Comrie, 1989, 1999; Woolford, 1999; Corbett, 2000; Croft, 2003), on their development. It also takes a look at the role of the emphatic and reflexive functions in the developments. RESULTS AND DISCUSSION According to the corpus, the first uses of personal clitic pronouns attached to xod belong to the third person singular and are not older that 14th or 15th centuries (8th or 9th in Hijri calendar). The third person singular reflexive pronoun xodaš, in both sequence of emergence and frequency in different centuries, overtly dominates the first and second reflexive pronouns. As illustrated in figure 1, first person also dominates the second person and singular is placed over plural in the hierarchy of number. Furthermore, according to the figure 2, the first cases of personal reflexive pronouns are used in the corpus in possessive constructions. Then emphatic function and later reflexive function are observed. Reference to PDF article Figure 1. Frequency of different personal reflexive pronouns in New Persian centuries Reference to PDF article Figure 2. Frequency of different functions of personal reflexive pronouns in New Persian centuries CONCLUSION The above mentioned results indicate that animacy does not play an important role in the indexation of Persian reflexive pronouns. Thus the development process is in consistency with the person hierarchies introduced by Greenberg (1966) and Comrie (1989; 1999) but portrays a counterexample to the hierarchy introduced by Faltz (1985), which considers a dominancy of second person over first person for reflexive markers. Moreover, the development follows the number hierarchy introduced by Woolford (1999). If, opposed to Davari (2016), we follow Moyne (1971) asserting that possessive uses of reflexive pronouns demonstrate an emphatic function in Persian, we conclude that the development of personal reflexive pronouns in New Persian follows the universal tendency introduced by König and Siemund (2000), indicating the preference for emphasis over reflexivity in the development of reflexive markers. | ||
کلیدواژهها [English] | ||
Typology, Reflexive pronouns, Emphatic, Pronominal clitics, New Persian | ||
سایر فایل های مرتبط با مقاله
|
||
مراجع | ||
| ||
آمار تعداد مشاهده مقاله: 557 تعداد دریافت فایل اصل مقاله: 288 |